#27 Enhancing Virtues: Caring (part 1)
J. Hughes
2014-12-09 00:00:00





15

According to IEET readers, what were the most stimulating stories of 2014? This month we’re answering that question by posting a countdown of the top 31 articles published this year on our blog (out of more than 1,000), based on how many total hits each one received.

The following piece was first published here on Aug 29, 2014,  and is the #27 most viewed of the year.

 







In this series:

Enhancing Virtues: Building the Virtues Control Panel

Enhancing Virtues: Positivity

Enhancing Virtues: Self-Control and Mindfulness

Enhancing Virtues: Caring (Part One)  (Part Two)   (Part Three)

Enhancing Virtues: Intelligence (Part One)   (Part Two)   (Part Three)   (Part Four)

Enhancing Virtues: Fairness (Part One)   (Part Two)   (Part Three)





 

To feel much for others and little for ourselves, that to restrain our selfish, and to indulge our benevolent affections, constitutes the perfection of human nature; and can alone produce among mankind that harmony of sentiments and passions in which consists their whole grace and propriety.

–Adam Smith, The Theory of Moral Sentiments pt i, ch i (1759)

The language of virtue and character has been in decline for at least a century, with the exception of the prosocial traits like kindness, fairness and compassion. The rise of the value on caring can be seen for instance in a study done on the frequency of virtue terms in books since 1900. [1] Three quarters of the terms, like virtue or character, declined in frequency, while compassion, fairness, tolerance and selflessness  were among the few that become more common. This is probably due to the spread of Enlightenement thought, which values caring and fairness over obedience to authority, self-control, in-group loyalty and deference to religion, as I will discuss in relation to Jon Haidt’s work in the next essay. 

As a result, in cultures influenced by the Enlightenment, caring for others has become almost synonymous with morality. In 2014 a large multinational team surveyed 2800 students from fourteen countries, and asked them to rate the importance of different virtues.[2] The only virtues that made it to the top of the list in all fourteen countries were the prosocial ones, honesty, respect and kindness. 

Caring Brains

Selection for Altruism  The existence of altruistic behavior in non-human animals has long been a puzzle to evolutionary biologists. Following Darwin and Dawkins they insisted that organisms are simply mechanisms for reproducing their own genes, so at best altruism was some kind of calculation on the part of genetics about the replication of the organism’s or its closely related genes. Putatively altruistic behavior might in fact be a selfish way of increasing social capital and future reproductive success. Or a suicidal sacrifice might be genes calculating that dying to save three siblings or half a dozen cousins is actually a statistically better deal.

But this dogma didn’t do much to explain non-genetically related altruism, and is now giving way to more acknowledgement of the mechanisms of “group selection,” natural selection at the level of groups.[3]  Genes for empathetic, altruistic and cooperative neurological mechanisms and behaviors increased the evolutionary fitness of the groups they belonged to, even if they didn’t increase the likelihood of that particular self-sacrificing animal reproducing. 

Emotional Empathy and Mirror Neurons  Research on rodents and primates suggests that at least one of these genetically-enabled empathic mechanisms is mirror neurons. Mirror neurons trigger sympathetic feelings in the parts of our brains that process touch, emotions, and actions when we observe those sensations, feelings or actions in others.  Human babies are distressed by seeing or hearing other infants cries, and, when they are able to, they try to comfort them.  They not only trigger sympathetic joy and distress when observing related animals of your own species, but probably also facilitate learning useful new behaviors through copying. These neurons are thought to help bond infants to their mothers by imitating facial expressions[4], and the imitative success of a newborn predicts their later emotional and cognitive development.[5] 

Cognitive Empathy and Theory of Mind  The more recent (50-100 thousand years old) cognitive architecture that empathy relies on is the prefrontal cortex’s ability to generate a model of what other people are thinking and feeling, a “theory of mind” (ToM). ToM is the result of a learning process about the relationship of other people’s experience and its relationship to one’s own.  This is sometimes called “cognitive empathy” to distinguish it from the more primitive forms of empathetic distress and mirror neuron facilitated “emotional empathy”.

The brain systems for emotive and cognitive empathy dynamically interact in most people to generate empathy and compassion.[8]  We can react viscerally to a scene in a movie in which someone is injured, while cognitively our empathy is tempered by knowing that no one was really hurt. Or we can cognitively understand that someone has suffered a tragedy even if they display no emotion to trigger our visceral reactions. We can also use cognitive empathy to generate compassion for people and animals that are very different from us, or that we can’t see.  Among the many effects of autism are impairments in the development of theory of mind and cognitive empathy; autists are just as emotionally responsive as “neurotypicals” but have a hard time understanding and predicting the feelings of others.  Psychopathy, on the other hand, is more a dysfunction of emotional empathy and the amygdala; psychopaths understand and can predict the emotions of others just fine, they just aren’t motivated to care about them.  A recent meta-analysis of 38 studies found that the amount of both cognitive and emotive empathy independently predicts criminal offending[9] (although it is important to note that psychopathy is a predictor of criminal behavior while autism is not).

Oxytocin  Emotional empathy, cognitive empathy and prosocial behavior are all moderately to highly inheritable according a recent review of evidence by Chakrabarti and Baron-Cohen, with emotional empathy having the strongest genetic correlates.[10] One of the nine candidate genetic variations Chakrabarti and Baron-Cohen identify as linked to differences in empathy are genes that determine how much of the neurochemical oxytocin is produced in the brain.  Oxytocin  is boosted naturally in the body when mothers interact with infants, stimulating the flow of breast milk, and during sex. Oxytocin enhances parts of the brain that attend to other people’s faces and emotions, and boosts the connection between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex.[11] Variations in oxytocin genes, and the amount of oxytocin in the brain, are associated with differences in both emotional and cognitive empathy,[12] prosocial behaviors[13], facial and emotional recognition skills[14], the personality trait of extraversion[15] and the incidence of autism[16] [17]  and psychopathic callousness[18] [19].

Because it is a safe and easily administered chemical, hundreds of experiements have been done on the behavioral and neurological effects of snorting oxytocin. Oxytocin enhancement generally increases interpersonal attentivenes, trust, constructive communication and prosocial behavior[20], at least between relatives and other in-group members. Oxytocin enhancement makes other people appear more attractive, and improves men’s sexual performance.[21] Married men given oxytocin get more rewards from their brain when looking at images of their wives’ faces,[22] and oxytocin also enhances positive feelings of parents towards infants. Oxytocin therapy is finding some moderate success as a treatment for autistic children’s attentiveness to faces and emotions.[23]

Testosterone  Genes that control testosterone are also identified by Chakrabarti and Baron-Cohen as linked to empathy. One theory about the etiology of autism is that it is Extreme Male Brain, triggered by exposure to high levels of testosterone during fetal development.[24]  Levels of testosterone are also linked to aggression, perception of threats and dominance behavior,[25] and the levels of testosterone have declined in the human species over the last 200,000 years contributing to a decline in violence.[26] The effects of testosterone are still poorly understood however. One study found for instance, that giving doses of testosterone to women and to men with low prenatal testosterone increased their cooperative behavior.[27] [28] 

Serotonin  A third set of genes linked to empathy code for the production of serotonin. The more serotonin people have the more they empathize with other people’s emotions, and variations in serotonin genes are associated with prosocial behavior because serotonin boosts the threat people feel from others’ distress. [29] Boosting serotonin with SSRIs reduces the willingness to punish others in games.[30]

Personality: Agreeableness These differences in prosocial brain structure and neurochemistry presumably underlie our personality settings for agreeableness and willingness to cooperate.[31]  The personality trait of agreeableness has been identified in chimpanzees[32], suggesting that we share these neurological variations in empathy and sociability settings. People who score high on agreeableness are more empathetic[33], trusting and prosocial throughout their lives.  

Part Two will be published Monday

 

References

[1] Kesebir P, Kesebir S. The cultural salience of moral character and virtue declined in twentieth century America. Journal of Positive Psychology. 2012;  7:6, 471-480. DOI: 10.1080/17439760.2012.715182

[2] van Oudenhoven JP, et al. Are virtues national, supranational, or universal? Springerplus. 2014; 2;3:223. doi: 10.1186/2193-1801-3-223.

[3] Nowak MA, Tarnita CE, Wilson EO. The evolution of eusociality. Nature. 2010; 466: 1057-1062. doi:10.1038/nature09205

[4] Suddendorf T, et al. Is newborn imitation developmentally homologous to later social-cognitive skills? Developmental Psychobiology. 2013: 52–58.

[5] Simpson EA, et al. The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. 2014; 369(1644): doi: 20130289

[8] Raz G, Jacob Y, Gonen T, et al. Cry for her or cry with her: Context-dependent dissociation of two modes of cinematic empathy reflected in network cohesion dynamics. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013;  doi: 10.1093/scan/nst052.

[9] Van Langen MAM, et al. The relation between empathy and offending: A meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 2014; 19(2): 179–189.

[10] Chakrabarti, B. Baron-Cohen S. Understanding the genetics of empathy and the autistic spectrum, in S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager- Flusberg, M. Lombardo. (eds). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Social Neuroscience. 2013; Oxford: Oxford University Press.

[11] Sripada CS, et al. Oxytocin enhances resting-state connectivity between amygdala and medial frontal cortex. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013; 16(2): 255-260.

[12] Wu N, Li Z, Su Y. The association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012; 138(3):  468–472.

[13] Wu N, Su Y. Oxytocin Receptor Gene Relates to Theory of Mind and Prosocial Behavior in Children. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2013; DOI: 10.1080/15248372.2013.858042

[14] Skuse DH, et al. Common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with human social recognition skills. PNAS. 2014; 11(5):  1987–1992.

[15] Andari E, et al. Oxytocin’s Fingerprint in Personality Traits and Regional Brain Volume. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:479–486.

[16] LoParo D, Waldman D. The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry. 2014; doi:10.1038/mp.2014.77.

[17] Parker KJ, et al. Plasma oxytocin concentrations and OXTR polymorphisms predict social impairments in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. PNAS. 2014; http://www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1402236111

[18] Dadds MR, et al. Polymorphisms in the oxytocin receptor gene are associated with the development of psychopathy. Development and Psychopathology. 2014; 26: 21–31.

[19] Dadds MR, et al. Methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene and oxytocin blood levels in the development of psychopathy. Development and Psychopathology. 2014;26: 33–40.

[20] Barrazza JA, McCullough ME, Ahmadi S, Zak PJ. Oxytocin infusion increases charitable donations regardless of monetary resources. Hormones and Behavior. 2011; 60: 148–151

[21] MacDonald K, Feifel D. Dramatic Improvement in Sexual Function Induced by Intranasal Oxytocin. J Sex Med. 2012;9:1407–1410.

[22] Scheele D, et al. Oxytocin enhances brain reward system responses in men viewing the face of their female partner. PNAS. 2013; www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1314190110

[23] Antonio P, et al. Oxytocin and Autism: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology. March 2014, 24(2): 54-68. doi:10.1089/cap.2013.0040.

[24] Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S. Fetal Testosterone in Mind: Implications for Autism. In Pfaff DW,  Christen Y. (eds.), Multiple Origins of Sex Differences in Brain, Research and Perspectives in Endocrine Interactions. 2013 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg.

[25] Goetz SMM, et al. Testosterone Rapidly Increases Neural Reactivity to Threat in Healthy Men: A Novel Two-Step Pharmacological Challenge Paradigm. Biological Psychiatry. 2014; 76(4): DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.016.

[26] Cieri RL, et al. Craniofacial Feminization, Social Tolerance, and the Origins of Behavioral Modernity. Current Anthropology. 2014; 55(4): 419-443.

[27] Eisenegger C, et al. Prejudice and truth about the effect of testosterone on human bargaining behaviour. Nature. 2010; 463: 356-359.

[28] Van Honk J, et al. New evidence on testosterone and cooperation. Nature. 2012; 485: E4-E5.

[29] Ratner KG, Way BM. Unselfish genes? The quest to uncover genomic influences on prosocial behavior. Social Neuroscience. 2013; 8(5): 397-399.

[30] Siegel JZ, Crockett MJ. How serotonin shapes moral judgment and behavior. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2013; 1299:  42–51.

[31] Cesarini D, et al. Heritability of cooperative behavior in the trust game. PNAS. 2008; 105(10): 3721–3726.

[32] Latzman RD, et al. Personality in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Exploring the Hierarchical Structure and Associations with the Vasopressin V1A Receptor Gene. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9(4): e95741. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0095741

[33] Mooradian TA, Davis M, Matzler K. Dispositional Empathy and the Hierarchical Structure of Personality. American Journal of Psychology. 2011; 124(11): 99-109.